Sign In · New? Sign Up
Learn More Psychology
Access every article, download
guides, audio and more.
Join Today
Psychologist World

Agreeableness

What is agreeableness and is it desirable as a personality trait?

Agreeableness

Agreeableness is one of the ‘big five’ personality factors or traits which has for decades been the subject of psychology research. A person who is highly agreeable will exhibit pro-social forms of behavior. They are more sociable, aim to please other people and are willing to help those in need. As a result, agreeable people tend to work well as part of a team. During arguments or times of conflict, they will seek to resolve, rather than prolong, confrontation.

Big Five Personality Traits

People with low levels of agreeableness scores are less altruistic. They are often less willing to co-operate with others, and are instead motivated to pursue their personal interests, rather than those of their group.

As with other personality traits, agreeableness remains fluid throughout a person’s life. Research has found that we tend to become more agreeable as we grow older (Donnelan et al, 2009).

Agreeableness and Disagreeableness

A tendency towards pro-social behavior can lead an agreeable person to be perceived as warm, sociable and ‘likeable amongst his or her peers’. They are friendly towards other people, tolerant of others and show more patience than those who are less agreeable. A person who is agreeable may be more trusting of others and their intentions.

Agreeable people also exhibit more altruistic behavior. They are generally thoughtful of, and more kind to, peers, are generous towards people in a disadvantaged position and are willing to help those who need it.

Whilst some people thrive during conflicts, people who are agreeable try to avoid or resolve such situations. They will often acts as the mediator or ‘peacemaker’ of a group, attempting to appease two arguing or conflicting parties.  

A person with a low level of agreeableness - someone who is disagreeable - displays less pro-social behavior. They tend to be more focused on their person needs, even when these conflict with the interests of others. Their peers may therefore view them as somewhat selfish in their behavior.

Low agreeableness can also result in a person being suspicious and distrusting of the motives of others. They may be more argumentative, and be less willing to engage in conversations with, or socialise with, unfamiliar people.

People with low levels of the agreeableness have been found to exhibit higher levels of ‘dark triad’ traits, a series of characteristics with negative associations, including Machiavellianism, narcissism and psychopathy (Jakobwitz and Egan, 2006).

A ‘Big Five’ Trait

A significant amount of research has been undertaken into agreeableness owing to it belonging to a group of five ‘super’ personality factors. Psychologists believe that these factors - which also include openness, conscientiousness, extraversion and neuroticism - can account for most of the differences observed in people’s personalities.

Agreeableness is included in numerous theories of personality, including the five factor model proposed by Robert McCrae and Paul Costa, Lewis Goldberg’s ‘Big Five’ factors and the HEXACO model developed by Michael Ashton and Kibeom Lee.

Such models follow a lexical approach to personality, which assumes that common traits can be described using existing terminology such as ‘agreeableness’, and that the observation of a trait necessitates that a language will develop a new term so that it may be discussed.

Measuring Agreeableness

Agreeableness is typically measured using self-report questionnaires, where a person is asked to review an adjective or statement and then report the degree to which it describes their personality on a Likert scale (e.g. ‘1 - Strongly Disagree’ to ‘5 - Strongly Agree’). Question inventories which include measures for agreeableness include the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and the International Personality Item Pool (Costa and McCrae, 1987).

The use of such questions to assess agreeableness allows a respondent to provide answers which provide a greater insight into their personality than the observation of his or her behavior might. However, questionnaires are also require respondents to be honest when providing answers.

Research has found that respondents often provide answers which they believe will project themselves positively, rather than reflecting their actual personalities. This distortion of responses with a view to more positive self-presentation is known as a social desirability bias.

Should I Be More Agreeable?

Traits of agreeableness are commonly viewed in a positive light, and are considered by many to be socially desirable. Indeed, the ‘likeability’ of an agreeable person and their willingness to cooperate with peers can help them to thrive in social situations.

Even from an early age, agreeableness can be an advantageous trait. In a study of the trait amongst students, researcher Lauri Jensen-Campbell found that children with higher levels of agreeableness tended to be subject to less bullying behavior than those who were low in agreeableness (Jensen-Campbell et al, 2002).

Yet, the interaction between childhood experiences and agreeableness is unclear.

A recent study of undergraduate students, published in the journal Frontiers in Psychology, suggests that experiences of “adversity” at home during childhood often led to participants reporting lower levels of agreeableness as adults. The researchers reported that, “...early adversity related to lower levels of agreeableness and engagement and to higher levels of anger/aggression and extrinsic focus” (Carver et al, 2014).

Moreover, agreeableness is not always advantageous. Whilst it can help a person working in a group, it is less helpful to those engaging in tasks that are confrontational in nature.

For instance, a teacher trying to gain the respect of students in a classroom may need to persuade them to comply with their directions, rather than to compromise with them.

References
  1. Donnellan, M. B. and Lucas, R. E. (2008). Age Differences in the Big Five Across the Life Span: Evidence from Two National Samples. Psychology and Aging. 23(3). 558-566.
  2. Jakobwitz, S. and Egan, V. (2006). The dark triad and normal personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences. 40(2). 331-339.
  3. McCrae, R. R. and Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the Five-Factor Model of Personality Across Instruments and Observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 52(1). 81-90.
  4. Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Adams, R., Perry, D. G., Workman, K. A., Furdella, J. Q. and Egan, S. K. (2002). Agreeableness, Extraversion, and Peer Relations in Early Adolescence: Winning Friends and Deflecting Aggression. Journal of Research in Personality. 363. 224-251.
  5. Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., McCullough, M. E., Forster, D. E and Joormann, J. (2014). Frontiers in Psychology. Retrieved from http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01357/full.
Most Read
Personality Quizzes
Advertisement
Self-Help Guides
Follow Psychologist World

More on Personality


Are You Authoritarian?

How Theodor Adorno's F-scale aimed to identify fascism and authoritarian...

Authoritarian Personality

More on Personality

Sign Up for Unlimited Access
Psychologist World
Join Psychologist World today for unlimited access to 2,200+ psychology theories, approaches, studies, experiments and guides:
  • Psychology approaches, theories and studies explained
  • Body Language Reading Guide
  • How to Interpret Your Dreams Guide
  • Self Hypnosis Downloads
  • Plus More Member Benefits

You May Also Like...